Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Extending the data
المؤلف:
David Odden
المصدر:
Introducing Phonology
الجزء والصفحة:
161-6
2-4-2022
1335
Extending the data
Verbs are subject to these same rules, as some additional data will show, and an analysis of verbs will provide additional support for aspects of this analysis. Hehe is a tone language, and while we have not been concerned with accounting for tone (and have not marked tones), in the following data, tones are marked, and can be predicted by rule. In analyzing these data, we want to account for the placement of the high tone (H), which is marked with an acute accent.
The morphology. These data indicate that all verbs begin with kú or something derivable from /kú/ by the rules already motivated, thus we assume that kú- is an inflectional prefix. In addition, all verbs end with the vowel a, which is probably a morpheme since it is unlikely that every root would end in exactly the same vowel. The stem of the word for ‘milk’ is probably -kam-. Various grammatical relations are expressed by suffixes standing between the stem and the suffix -a, such as -il- ‘for,’ -an- ‘each other,’ -j- ‘make,’ -w- ‘passive’: the objects ‘us’ and ‘them’ are marked by the prefixes -tu- and -va- between the prefix kú and the verb stem.
Phonological rules. Looking at the last three roots, which are vowel-initial, the prefixes kú-, tu-, and va- are subject to the rules motivated on the basis of nouns, where /u/ becomes [w] before a vowel, but deletes after a consonant and before a round vowel (so, /ku-oogopa/ ! kwoogopa ! [kóogópa]); the sequence vo becomes wo (/ku-va-oogopa/ ! kuvoogopa ! [kúwoogópa]). The change of /v/ to w is also seen in examples such as kútowá and kúlawá, coming (apparently) from /ku-tov-w-a/ and /ku-lav-w-a/.
The rule of v-rounding would derive kútowwá and kúlawwá, and the actual phonetic forms can be accounted for based on that intermediate form by Glide Degemination.
One additional segmental process of vowel harmony is motivated by the above examples. The benefactive suffix retains its underlying high vowel in forms such as kúkam-íl-a, kúsan-íl-a, and kúfwiim-íl-a, but that vowel assimilates in height to a preceding mid vowel in examples such as kútov-él-a, kwéend-él-a, and kóogop-él-a. This motivates the following vowel harmony rule:
Regarding tone, most examples have an H tone on the second-to-last vowel of the word (this may be the second part of a long vowel in the penultimate syllable, or the only vowel of a short penultimate syllable), which can be accounted for by the following rule.
In some verbs, this H is missing – see kúkama, kúsana, kútova. Applying this tone assignment rule to these forms would result in outputs such as *kúkáma, *kúsána, *kútóva, with H tones on adjacent vowels. Since our examples contain no cases of consecutive H-toned vowels, we may assume a rule along the following lines.
What about the columns with the suffixes -j- ‘make’ and -w- ‘passive,’ which have word-final H, not penult H? We expect *kúkalaángwa. But if these two suffixes are underlyingly i and u, then the underlying form of kúkalaangwá would be /kúkalaang-u-a/. H tone would be assigned to the penultimate vowel under that assumption, giving kúkalaangúa. However, we already know that there is a rule of glide formation which would turn u and i into w and y before vowels, a rule which has obviously applied in these forms. Since only syllabic elements can bear tones, the tone on the penultimate vowel apparently shifts to the final syllable, where it can be pronounced.
الاكثر قراءة في Phonology
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
