Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Simple
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Passive and Active
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Assessment
BRAIN LATERALISATION
المؤلف:
John Field
المصدر:
Psycholinguistics
الجزء والصفحة:
P46
2025-08-03
44
BRAIN LATERALISATION
The view that one of the hemispheres of the brain has or develops a special responsibility for language. In most human beings, language appears to be particularly associated with the left hemisphere. Evidence comes from:
Brain damage. When the left hemisphere is damaged by an accident, a stroke or invasive surgery, it often has a serious impact upon the victim’s language.
Commisurotomy. In this operation, the corpus callosum joining the two hemispheres was severed to reduce the effects of epilepsy. Patients who underwent the operation could name objects in their right field of vision (connecting with the left hemisphere) but not those in their left field of vision.
Wada injections of sodium amytol were once used to de-activate one side of the brain prior to surgery, in order to ascertain which hemisphere was the dominant one for language. It was usually the left one.
Dichotic listening. When different messages are presented to the two ears, the right ear (the ear that links to the left hemisphere) usually overrides the left. The right ear appears to be dominant for speech generally but the left for music, rhythm and intonation.
There appears to be a partial relationship between lateralisation and handedness. Rather fewer left-handers than right-handers appear to have a dominant left hemisphere for language.
When damage occurs to the left hemisphere of the brain before the age of five, the victim’s powers of speech sometimes recover completely, and language dominance appears to re-establish itself in the right hemisphere. Some researchers have concluded that the language faculty may be generally distributed at birth, and that lateralisation occurs gradually as part of maturation. The hypothesis has been linked to the notion of a critical period. First language acquisition, it is argued, has to take place during the period of lateralisation if complete command of L1 is to be achieved. If acquisition is delayed, the result may be the inadequate mastery observed in cases of language deprivation.
However, the ‘plasticity’ hypothesis has been challenged by evidence that a degree of left-hemisphere lateralisation exists from birth. Furthermore, recent studies have identified cases of infants who have not recovered some aspects of their linguistic competence after left-brain damage, and of adults who seem to have recovered their language by relocating it to their right hemisphere.
In addition, we now have more evidence about right-hemisphere damage. While damage to the left hemisphere affects primary language functions such as syntax and lexis, damage to the right appears to affect the processing of discourse. It also seems that, while phoneme-level processing is chiefly the prerogative of the left hemisphere, the processing of suprasegmental features, especially prosody, takes place on the right. One current view of the lateralisation issue is thus that the left hemisphere specialises in more rapid language operations, and the right in those connected to higher-level meaning processes. This division of functions makes sense when the brain has to deal with two closely associated processes such as processing phonemes (left side) and prosody (right side). It also accords with evidence from modern imaging techniques showing that language is much more widely distributed in the brain than the original lateralisation hypothesis supposed.
It may be that the human brain is more flexible than once believed, even in adulthood when the critical period is long over. In the exceptional circumstances of simultaneous translation, some profes sionals appear to distribute their two operating languages between the two hemispheres– usually reserving the left for the native language and the right for the second. In this way, they keep their two languages apart and manage to cope with the lack of synchronicity between the incoming auditory signal and the outgoing translation. Again, the key seems to be the need to keep two closely associated operations distinct.
It was once believed that lateralisation might be peculiar to human beings and afford an explanation of why language is unique to our species. However, an enlarged left hemisphere has been found to occur in other species, including birds. Frogs have a dominant hemisphere which appears to be associated with croaking. This suggests that, across species, the larger side of the brain has special functions related to vocalisation and the processing of rapid auditory stimuli.
See also: Brain: localisation, Critical period, Deprivation
Further reading: Aitchison (1998); Deacon (1997); Dingwall (1998); Obler and Gjerlow (1999); Springer and Deutsch (1997)
الاكثر قراءة في Linguistics fields
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
