المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
المرجع الألكتروني للمعلوماتية

English Language
عدد المواضيع في هذا القسم 6095 موضوعاً
Grammar
Linguistics
Reading Comprehension

Untitled Document
أبحث عن شيء أخر

حفص بن البختري
7-9-2016
قصص عن الزهد.
2023-12-19
الصدقة
14-6-2017
عناصر المناخ – الحرارة - كيف تنتقل الحرارة بين الاجسام - الإشعاع
29/11/2022
ابو العباس وقيام بني العباس
27-6-2017
حملة نادر شاه الثانية على العراق.
2024-10-29

Reflection: Recognitional deixis or thingamy  
  
206   03:19 مساءً   date: 26-4-2022
Author : Jonathan Culpeper and Michael Haugh
Book or Source : Pragmatics and the English Language
Page and Part : 40-2


Read More
Date: 6-5-2022 568
Date: 4-5-2022 317
Date: 26-5-2022 148

Reflection: Recognitional deixis or thingamy

A specific phenomenon that it is strongly dependent on common ground concerns nominal expressions such as thingamy, thingamajig, thingamabob, whojar, whatsit, what’s-his-name, what-d’you-call-it, and you-know-what. Such expressions, unlike the definite noun phrases discussed earlier, provide little descriptive information to help one work out what is being talked about. Yet, in the shared context of a dinner, an expression such as thingamy is likely to be taken as a reference to the pepper mill on the table, and was equally likely to have been used in the expectation that this reference would be assigned. Recognitional deixis is a label coined by Enfield (2003) for such items. It acknowledges that they behave like deictic expressions picking out items relative to the speaker’s here and now, but also that they rely heavily on common ground to be successfully “recognized”.

Why might we use such words? Consider these examples of recent usage of thingamy taken from the Oxford English Corpus:

Although you do not have access to the full context, it is quite easy to see that they are often used in contexts where specialized lexis would be required (e.g. technical items or specialized cuisine), or where there may be difficulty in identifying an appropriate word (e.g. dealing with the sensitivities of a diplomatic wrangle), or where there are restrictions on the obvious term (e.g. labelling a cleaning product without actually using the trademark name). In context, it is highly likely that these will present few difficulties, because the participants have a common ground. Additionally, it is not the case that such expressions present participants with no descriptive information at all; in fact, they offer constraints on interpretations. This point is spelt out by Enfield (2003). He suggests, for example, that the speaker’s message in using what’s-his-name could be defined as: someone (male); I can’t say this person’s name now; by saying what’s-his-name I think you’ll know who I’m thinking of (2003:105). This contrasts with, for example, you-know-what, which “may have both ‘avoidance’ and ‘conspiratorial’ functions, whereby a speaker deliberately avoids saying a certain word, either to prevent potentially overhearing third parties from understanding, and/or to create exclusive air between interlocutors” (2003:106).