المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
المرجع الألكتروني للمعلوماتية

English Language
عدد المواضيع في هذا القسم 6149 موضوعاً
Grammar
Linguistics
Reading Comprehension

Untitled Document
أبحث عن شيء أخر المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
تـشكيـل اتـجاهات المـستـهلك والعوامـل المؤثـرة عليـها
2024-11-27
النـماذج النـظريـة لاتـجاهـات المـستـهلـك
2024-11-27
{اصبروا وصابروا ورابطوا }
2024-11-27
الله لا يضيع اجر عامل
2024-11-27
ذكر الله
2024-11-27
الاختبار في ذبل الأموال والأنفس
2024-11-27

هل الكون تقلبات فراغية؟
24-1-2023
أداء الحقوق الزوجية
2024-10-19
كيف يبدو الوسواس القهري؟
18-9-2019
مفاعل ماء يغلي boiling water reactor
4-2-2018
مقدمة عن الثرموداينميك Introduction to thermodynamic
2024-03-23
المبيدات المايكروبية والجينية
2023-05-11

Thematic roles  
  
1903   11:44 صباحاً   date: 24-1-2023
Author : Andrew Radford
Book or Source : Minimalist Syntax
Page and Part : 250-7


Read More
Date: 2023-10-11 686
Date: 17-2-2022 1879
Date: 2023-11-10 749

Thematic roles

We concluded that the argument structure of clauses is directly reflected in the internal syntactic structure of verb phrases. However, there is an important sense in which it is not enough simply to say that in a sentence such as (18b) The police have arrested the suspect the verb arrest is a predicate which has two arguments – the internal argument the suspect and the external argument the police. After all, such a description fails to account for the fact that these two arguments play very different semantic roles in relation to the act of arrest – i.e. it fails to account for the fact that the police are the individuals who perform the act (and hence get to verbally and physically abuse the suspect), and that the suspect is the person who suffers the consequences of the act (e.g. being manhandled, handcuffed, thrown into the back of a windowless vehicle and beaten up). Hence, any adequate account of argument structure should provide a description of the semantic role which each argument plays.

In research spanning half a century – beginning with the pioneering work of Gruber (1965), Fillmore (1968) and Jackendoff (1972) – linguists have attempted to devise a universal typology of the semantic roles played by arguments in relation to their predicates. In the table in (22) below are listed a number of terms used to describe some of these roles (the convention being that terms denoting semantic roles are CAPITALIZED), and for each role an informal gloss is given, together with an illustrative example (in which the italicized expression has the semantic role specified):

We can illustrate how the terminology in (22) can be used to describe the semantic roles played by arguments in terms of the following examples

Given that – as we see from these examples – the THEME role is a central one, it has become customary over the past two decades to refer to the relevant semantic roles as thematic roles; and since the Greek letter θ (= theta) corresponds to th in English and the word thematic begins with th, it has also become standard practice to abbreviate the expression thematic role to θ -role (pronounced theeta-role by some and thayta-role by others). Using this terminology, we can say (e.g.) that in (23a) the FBI is the AGENT argument of the predicate arrested, and that Larry Luckless is the THEME argument of arrested.

Thematic relations (like AGENT and THEME) have been argued to play a central role in the description of a range of linguistic phenomena. For example, it has been argued that the distribution of certain types of adverb is thematically determined. Thus, Gruber (1976) argues that adverbs like deliberately can only be used to modify AGENT arguments:

Likewise, Fillmore (1972, p. 10) argues that the adverb personally can only be associated with EXPERIENCER arguments:

In a similar vein, Fillmore (1968, p. 10) argues that only constituents with the same thematic function can be coordinated:

And Jackendoff (1972) argues at length that a number of constraints on passive structures can be accounted for in thematic terms. For example, he argues (1972, p. 44) that the ill-formedness of passive sentences like:

is attributable to violation of the following condition (formulated in thematic terms):

The hierarchy referred to in (28) is that in (29) below:

Jackendoff maintains that the by-phrase in both examples in (27) is a THEME argument of the relevant verb, whereas the superficial subject is a LOCATIVE argument. Since THEME is lower on the hierarchy (29) than LOCATIVE, sentences like (27) violate the condition (28) and so are ungrammatical.

If we look closely at the examples in (23), we see a fairly obvious pattern emerging. Each of the bracketed argument expressions in (23) carries one and only one θ-role, and no two arguments of any predicate carry the same θ-role. Chomsky (1981) suggested that these thematic properties of arguments are the consequence of a principle of Universal Grammar traditionally referred to as the θ-criterion, and outlined in (30) below:

A principle along the lines of (30) has been assumed (in some form or other) in much subsequent work.

However, a question which arises from (30) is how θ-roles are assigned to arguments. It seems clear that in V-bar constituents of the form verb+ complement, the thematic role of the complement is determined by the semantic properties of the verb. As examples like (23a–c) illustrate, the associated with complements is often that of THEME (though this is not always the case – e.g. the complement me of the verb bother in Personally, it doesn’t bother me has the thematic role of EXPERIENCER). However, the question of how subjects are assigned θ-role is more complex.

Marantz (1984, pp. 23ff.) and Chomsky (1986a, pp. 59–60) argue that although verbs directly assign θ-role to their internal arguments (i.e. complements), it is not the verb but rather the whole verb+ complement (i.e. V-bar) expression which determines the θ-role assigned to its external argument. The evidence they adduce in support of this conclusion comes from sentences such as:

Although the subject of the verb threw in both (31a) and (31b), John plays a different thematic role in the two sentences – that of AGENT in the case of threw a ball, but that of EXPERIENCER in threw a fit. Likewise, although the subject of the verb broke in both (32a) and (32b), John plays the role of AGENT in (32a) but that of EXPERIENCER on the most natural (accidental arm-breaking) interpretation of (32b). From examples such as these, Marantz and Chomsky conclude that the thematic role of the subject is not determined by the verb alone, but rather is compositionally determined by the whole verb+ complement structure – i.e. by V-bar. On this view, a verb assigns a θ-role directly to its internal argument, but only indirectly (as a compositional function of the semantic properties of the overall V-bar) to its external argument. To use the relevant technical terminology, we can say that predicates directly θ -mark their complements, but indirectly θ -mark their subjects/specifiers.

A related observation is that auxiliaries seem to play no part in determining the assignment of θ -roles to subjects. For example, in sentences such as:

the thematic role of the subject he is determined purely by the choice of V-bar constituent (i.e. whether it is throw the ball or throw a fit), and is not affected in any way by the choice of auxiliary. Clearly, any theory of θ-marking should offer us a principled answer to questions such as the following: how are θ-roles assigned? Why do some constituents (e.g. verbs) play a key role in θ-marking, while others (e.g. auxiliaries) do not?

We can provide a principled answer to these questions in the following terms. Let us assume that θ-roles are assigned to arguments via merger with a predicative expression (i.e. an expression headed by an item which functions as a predicate – e.g. a verb). In the light of this observation, consider our earlier sentence (18b) The police have arrested the suspect. Since the verb arrested is a predicate which selects a THEME complement, the complement the suspect will be assigned the θ-role of THEME argument of arrest when the verb merges with its complement. Since arrest is a predicate which (in addition to requiring a THEME complement) also requires an AGENT external argument, the subject the police will be assigned the θ-role of AGENT argument of arrest when it merges with the V-bar arrested the suspect. The resulting VP the police arrested the suspect is then merged with the auxiliary have to form the T-bar have the police arrested the suspect. Because a finite T has an [EPP] feature requiring it to have a specifier, the subject the police raises to spec-TP, deriving The police have the police arrested the suspect. However, the subject the police does not receive any θ -role from the auxiliary have, since auxiliaries are not predicates (unlike main verbs) and hence do not θ - mark their subjects. The resulting TP is ultimately merged with a null declarative complementizer to derive the structure associated with (18b) The police have arrested the suspect.

Our discussion here suggests that thematic considerations lend further support to the VP-Internal Subject Hypothesis. By positing that subjects originate internally within VP, we can arrive at a unitary and principled account of θ-marking in terms of sisterhood, in that an argument is θ-marked by a predicative expression which is its sister: e.g. the verb arrested in (21) θ-marks its sister argument (complement) the suspect, and the V-bar arrested the suspect θ-marks its sister (subject) argument the police.