Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Degree modification on pre- and postnominal adjectives
المؤلف:
VIOLETA DEMONTE
المصدر:
Adjectives and Adverbs: Syntax, Semantics, and Discourse
الجزء والصفحة:
p84-C4
2025-04-12
118
Degree modification on pre- and postnominal adjectives
Of course, gradability is not a property which distinguishes between different logical types of adjectives. There are modal adjectives that are gradable: el muy possible acuerdo ‘the very possible agreement’ (but: ∗el muy supuesto asesino ‘the very alleged murderer’) or even event/deictic modifiers which accept degree modification: el muy anterior suceso ‘the very much earlier event.’ However, for the sake of my argument in this work, I will consider gradability as a property of basically two classes of adjectives: pure qualitative ones (los ojos tan limpios ‘such clear eyes,’ el niño menos feliz ‘the least happy boy,’ los más altos cipreses ‘the tallest cypresses,’ la muy seca piel de la niña ‘the girl’s very dry skin’) and deverbal ones (el muy discutido asunto ‘the much debated topic,’ el área completamente protegida ‘the fully protected area’).
Kennedy and McNally (2005) have convincingly argued that gradable adjectives can be partitioned into two semantic classes: relative vs. absolute adjectives. These two classes capture the fact that adjectives, by virtue of their lexical features, have different scalar properties. Absolute adjectives like awake, open, full, or straight have a non-context-dependent standard of comparison:
“they simply require their arguments to possess some minimal [or maximal] degree of the gradable property they introduce”. Relative adjectives like tall or expensive have a context-dependent standard: there is a contextual standard of comparison. In other words, absolute adjectives have a “closed” scale structure while relative adjectives have an “open” scale structure.
There are many interesting properties which differentiate these two types of adjectives.1 Here, I will concentrate only on the fact that contextual standard of comparison and scale structure affect the grammatical behavior of the degree modifiers which can be applied to gradable adjectives. According to the aforementioned authors, degree modifiers like very, much, and well/completely are sensitive to standard type and scale structure. More strictly, the degree modifier very is restricted to relative adjectives, while absolute adjectives, in normal usage, reject modification by this adverb:
In contrast, degree modifiers like well or proportional modifiers like completely combine with adjectives that have closed scales (2a) and (2b), but not with adjectives that have open scales (8c).
Let us consider now the two series of Spanish adjectives in (3). Those in (3a) are relative adjectives with an open scale and they accept modification by muy ‘very’ but not by the proportional modifier completamente ‘completely.’ Those in (3b) are absolute adjectives with a closed scale such that they can be modified by completamente ‘completely’.2
When relative adjectives with an open scale (3a) are modified by muy, this modifier boosts the (contextual) standard of the property with respect to the objects to which the adjective applies (Kennedy and McNally 2005). When absolute adjectives are modified by completamente this modifier fixes the degree of the property at an endpoint in the structure of the scale that a gradable adjective uses as a basis for ordering objects in its domain.3
Both types of adjectives can precede or follow N in Spanish DPs. However, the occurrence of both types of adjectives in pre- and postnominal position shows restrictions that depend precisely on the presence of the aforementioned degree modifiers. (In the following judgments, I disregard generic contexts where these distinctions do not hold.)
Observe the facts in (4)–(9). They show that there is a kind of complementary distribution between the two classes: relative adjectives modified by muy are felicitous in prenominal position; however, such a modification sounds awkward – or has to be qualified – when they are postnominal (examples 4, 5, and 6). In contrast, absolute adjectives modified by completamente are almost impossible prenominally, their standard position being the postnominal one (examples 7, 8, and 9) (of course NPs containing prenominal adjectives preceded by completamente are perfectly grammatical if the adverb is removed).
I consider it reasonable to assert that such contrastive behavior constitutes further evidence that pre- and postnominal adjectives have different semantic relations to the N they modify. I am unable to go as far as to establish exactly why fixing of a value on the scale (the function of completamente) helps to fulfill the restrictive function of postnominal adjectives and, at the same time, makes the adjective invalid to be an NR modifier. Perhaps the reason is that absolute adjectives modified by degree adverbs become stage-level predicates and stage-level predicates are not possible prenominally but only postnominally.
In contrast, it is more evident, at least intuitively, in what sense relative adjectives modified by muy help to delineate the central property expressed by the adjective: if the standard of the property is above the normal standard for the N, this property is more likely to be considered central or distinguished.
1 See Kennedy and McNally (2005) for a detailed analysis of relative and absolute adjectives.
2 When modified by very in special contexts, a fact that I do not consider here, they probably change their scalar properties and denote the average degree of the property of an object, thus behaving like relative adjectives.
3 Of course, completamente has other uses which are equivalent to very. Such cases will not be considered here.